Visit the James McKenna Library
Mothers and infants sleeping side by side, also known as cosleeping, is the evolved context of human infant sleep development. Until very recent times, for all human beings, it constituted a prerequisite for infant survival; outside of the Western industrialized context, for the majority of contemporary people, it still does. Because the human infant's body continues to be adapted only to the mother's body, cosleeping with nighttime breastfeeding remains clinically significant and potentially lifesaving.
This is because, of all mammals, humans are born the least neurologically mature (25 percent of adult brain volume), develop the most slowly, and are the most dependent for the longest period of time for nutritional, social, and emotional support, as well as for transportation. Indeed, in the early phases of human infancy, social care is synonymous with physiological regulation. That is, holding, carrying, and/or caressing an infant, and emitting odors and breath in his or her proximity, induce increased body temperature, less crying, greater heart rate variability, fewer apneas, lower stress levels, increased glucose storage, and greater daily growth.1
Moreover, since the content of human milk is relatively low in fat and protein and high in sugar, which is metabolized quickly, and since human infants are unable to locomote on their own, continuous contact and carrying, with frequent breastfeeding day and night, is required. Thus, any biological scientific study that attempts to understand "normal," species-wide, human infant sleep patterns without considering the vital role of nighttime contact in the form of breastfeeding and maternal proximity must be considered inadequate, misleading, and/or fundamentally flawed.2
Cosleeping: The Importance of Taxonomic Distinctions
Much of the controversy surrounding the question of the safety of mother-infant cosleeping involves the ways in which investigators define and conceptualize it. Cosleeping is not, as the Consumer Product Safety Commission (CPSC) assumes, a single, coherent practice. Rather, it is best thought of as a generic, diverse class of sleeping arrangements composed of many different "types" of practices, each of which requires proper description and characterization before the issue of safety and "outcomes" can be understood.
A safe cosleeping environment must provide the infant with the opportunity to "sense" and respond to the caregiver's signals and cues, such as the mother's smells, breathing sounds and movements, infant-directed speech, invitations to breastfeed, touches, and any "hidden" sensory stimuli, whether intended or not.3 Moreover, to be designated "safe," the physical and social cosleeping environment must involve a willing and active caregiver who chooses to cosleep specifically to nurture, feed, or be close to the infant in order to monitor or protect him or her.
The cosleeping environment also must be carefully constructed to avoid known hazardous conditions, recently revealed by epidemiological studies.4 Dangerous types of cosleeping include sleeping with infants on sofas or couches, bedsharing with mothers who smoke, and positioning toddlers next to infants. Parents or caregivers desensitized by drugs or alcohol create an unsafe cosleeping environment. Other dangerous cosleeping environments occur when an infant sleeps with a larger person on a soft mattress or is placed on large pillows in a bed with a parent.5,6,7
While all forms of bedsharing are examples of cosleeping, bedsharing is only one of perhaps hundreds of different ways to cosleep practiced around the world. For example, some parents in Latin America, the Philippines, and Vietnam sleep with their infant in a hammock, or place the infant in a hammock to sleep next to them, while they sleep on mats or beds. Some parents place their infant in a wicker basket and put the basket on a bed, between the parents. Other parents sleep next to their infants on bamboo or straw mats or on futons (as in Japan). Some place their infant on a cradleboard, keeping the infant within arm's reach; others cosleep by roomsharing, having the infant sleep on a different surface, such as in a crib or bassinet, which is kept next to the parental bed, within arm's reach.
Cosleeping Has Not Outlived Its Biological Usefulness
Although forms of infant sleeping vary enormously from culture to culture, the potentially beneficial physiological regulatory effects of maternal contact on human infants during sleep do not. Up to one degree of temperature can be lost when a newborn human is removed from the mother's stomach following birth, even when the infant is placed in an incubator with ambient temperatures set to match the mother's body temperature.8 Richard found that among 11- to 16-week-old infants, solitary-sleeping infants exhibited lower average axillary skin temperatures than breastfeeding infants sharing a bed with their mothers.9 Thoman and Graham discovered that even mechanical breathing teddy bears placed next to apnea-prone human newborns have the effect of reducing apneas by as much as 60 percent, in addition to physically drawing the infant subjects to sleep in direct contact.10 Moreover, when resting on their mothers' (or fathers') chests, skin-to-skin, both premature and full-term infants breathe more regularly, use energy more efficiently, grow faster, and experience less stress.11,12,13
Clinical Outcomes Depend on How Cosleeping Is Practiced
Exactly how cosleeping may be beneficial or dangerous to the infant varies as a function of the particular social and physical environment (family circumstances) within which it is expressed. This is why there is no single outcome associated with forms of cosleeping, especially in urban Western cultures, and why there is so much debate about whether cosleeping, especially in the form of bedsharing, is safe.
For example, in industrialized urban societies, among middle- to upper-class families where bedsharing and breastfeeding occur among nonsmoking mothers, infant mortality, including deaths from SIDS, is low. The most recent international study of childcare practices in relationship to SIDS rates, conducted by the SIDS Global Task Force, shows dramatically that low SIDS awareness and low SIDS rates are associated with the highest cosleeping-bedsharing rates (see Figure 1). At the most recent International SIDS Meeting in Auckland, New Zealand, Sankaran et al. presented data from Saskatchewan, Canada, showing that where breastfeeding and forms of cosleeping coexist, SIDS deaths are reduced.14 This finding is consistent with a study in South Africa indicating that bedsharing babies have higher survival rates than solitary-sleeping babies.15 In Hong Kong, where cosleeping is the norm, SIDS rates are among the lowest in the world.16,17 The same is true in Japan, where rates of not only SIDS but infant mortality in general are among the lowest in the world, according to the Japan SIDS Family Organization's 1999 report.18
Moreover, as shown in Figures 2-5, during a span of about four years in Japan, where maternal smoking has decreased while breastfeeding, cosleeping, and supine (faceup) infant sleep have increased, SIDS rates have decreased-the exact opposite of what bedsharing critics would predict. In many other Asian cultures where cosleeping is the norm, including China, Vietnam, Cambodia, and Thailand, SIDS is either unheard of or rare.19,20,21 In one study conducted in Australia, an immigrant Vietnamese mother was told about SIDS, with which she was unfamiliar. She said, "The custom of being with the baby must prevent this disease. If you are sleeping with your baby, you always sleep lightly. You notice if his breathing changes.... Babies should not be left alone." Another Vietnamese mother added, "Babies are too important to be left alone with nobody watching them."22
Of 40 Chinese women interviewed at Guagzho University Hospital by SIDS researcher Elizabeth Wilson, more than 66 percent of new mothers intended to have their infants sleep with them in the marital bed, and the rest of her sample planned to have the infant sleep alongside the bed. One informant represented many when she stated that the baby is "too little to sleep alone" and that cosleeping "makes babies happy."23
In contrast, in Western urban subgroups, cosleeping is associated with increased risks to the infant, especially but not exclusively when it occurs in association with maternal smoking, drug or alcohol use, chaotic lifestyles, lack of education and opportunities, prone sleeping, and other dangerous factors.24 For example, bedsharing deaths (which often erroneously include couch-sleeping deaths in the CPSC data bank) are especially high in the US among poor African Americans living in large cities such as Chicago, Cleveland, Washington, D.C., and St. Louis-the four cities from which data used to argue against the safety of all cosleeping, regardless of circumstances, emerge.25,26 Moreover, epidemiological studies show consistently across cultures that among economically deprived, indigenous groups, such as the Maori in New Zealand, Aborigines in Australia, Cree in Canada, and Aleuts in Alaska, bedsharing and other forms of cosleeping can be associated also with increased risks to infants and increased infant deaths.27,28
The SIDS Global Task Force accounts for these differences in bedsharing outcomes in a way consistent with my own view, pointing to factors such as parental smoking, drug and alcohol use, infants sleeping prone on soft mattresses, infants sleeping alone on adult beds with gaps or ledges around the bed frame or between the mattress and a wall or piece of furniture, dangerous furniture or furniture arrangements, and infants sleeping next to toddlers or on sofas with obese adults.
Perhaps it is best to conceptualize outcomes related to bedsharing in terms of a benefits-risks continuum (see Figure 6). For example, if mothers elect to bedshare for purposes of nurturing and breastfeeding and are knowledgeable about safety precautions (e.g., use stiff mattresses, do not over-wrap the infant, lay babies supine, etc.), we can expect that bedsharing will be protective or reduce SIDS risks. But when bedsharing is not chosen as a childcare strategy but rather is a necessity because there is no other place to put the baby, and mothers smoke, take drugs, and do not place an adult in between a toddler and a baby sharing a bed, increased risk of SIDS or asphyxiation can be predicted.
Solitary Infant Sleep: A Historical Novelty
Emotions, designed by natural selection and controlled by the limbic system of the brain, motivate infants and children to protest sleep isolation from parents by crying. These emotions undoubtedly evolved to ameliorate what was throughout our evolution a life-threatening situation: separation from the caregiver.29
In recent decades, Western childcare strategies have favored early infant autonomy. Health professionals teach that parents should condition infants to sleep alone throughout the night with minimal parental intervention, including breastfeedings (according to some advice givers, the fewer number of breastfeeds the better).30,31 Parents are encouraged by some health professionals to "train" their infants to "soothe themselves back to sleep." Pediatric sleep advisers say that infants should never be permitted to fall asleep at the breast or in the mother's arms, even though this is the very context within which the infant's "falling asleep" evolved. As many parents will attest, this advice proves highly problematic.
The exaggerated fear of suffocating an infant while cosleeping may stem, in part, from Western cultural history. During the last 500 years, many economically destitute women in Paris, Brussels, Munich, and London (to name but a few locales) confessed to Catholic priests of having murdered their infants by overlying, in order to control family size. The priests threatened excommunication, fines, or imprisonment-and banned infants from parental beds.32,33
The legacy of this particular historical condition in the Western world probably converged with other changing social mores and customs (the emphasis on privacy, self-reliance, and individualism), providing a philosophical foundation for contemporary cultural beliefs and making it easier to find dangers associated with cosleeping than to find (or assume) hidden benefits. The proliferation throughout Europe of the idea of romantic love, coupled with the belief in the importance of the husband-wife relationship, also may have promoted separate sleeping quarters. This physical separation, especially of the father from his children, also was seen as maximizing the father's ability to dispense religious training and to display moral authority.
Cosleeping and Solitary Sleeping Arrangements: Effects on Children
As I have noted elsewhere, the first published studies of people who coslept as infants contradict conventional Western assumptions that cosleeping leads to negative psychological, emotional, and social outcomes later in life.34,35,36 A recent cross-sectional study of middle-class English children shows that children who never slept in their parents' beds were more likely to be rated by teachers and parents as "harder to control" and "less happy" and exhibited a greater number of tantrums. Children never permitted to bedshare also were more fearful than those who slept in their parents' beds.37
Other findings point to further advantages of cosleeping over solitary sleeping. A survey of college-age subjects found that males who coslept with their parents between birth and five years of age had significantly higher self-esteem, experienced less guilt and anxiety, and reported greater frequency of sex. Males who coslept between 6 and 11 years of age also had higher self-esteem. For women, cosleeping during childhood was associated with less discomfort about physical contact and affection as adults.38 Another study found that women who coslept as children had higher self-esteem than those who did not.39 Indeed, cosleeping appears to promote confidence, self-esteem, and intimacy, possibly by reflecting an attitude of parental acceptance (see Figures 7 and 8).
A study of 86 children on military bases revealed that cosleeping children received higher evaluations of their comportment from teachers than solitary-sleeping children and that they were underrepresented in psychiatric-care populations compared with children who did not cosleep. The authors stated:
Contrary to expectations, those children who had not had previous professional attention for emotional or behavioral problems co-slept more frequently than did children who were known to have had psychiatric intervention and lower parental ratings of adaptive functioning. The same finding occurred in a sample of boys one might consider Oedipal visitors (e.g., three-year-old and older boys who sleep with their mothers in the absence of the father)-a finding which directly opposes traditional psychoanalytic thought.40The largest and possibly most systematic study to date, involving more than 1,400 subjects from five ethnic groups in Chicago and New York, found far more positive than negative adult outcomes for individuals who coslept as children. The results were the same for almost all the ethnic groups (African Americans and Puerto Ricans in New York; Puerto Ricans, Dominicans, and Mexicans in Chicago). An especially robust finding, one that cut across all ethnic groups, was that cosleepers exhibited a greater feeling of satisfaction with life.41
Physiological Studies of Mother-Infant Pairs
A study at the University of California-Irvine School of Medicine quantified differences in the sleep behavior and physiology of 70 Latina mothers and infants. More than 200 eight-hour polysonographic recordings were made of mothers and their infants sharing a bed or sleeping apart in adjacent rooms over three successive nights. We specifically compared how the solitary sleep environment and the bedsharing environment affected two kinds of mother-infant pairs: those who routinely bedshared at home and those who routinely slept apart.
In randomly assigned order, each mother-infant pair spent two nights sleeping in their routine (home) sleeping condition and one night sleeping in the nonroutine condition; that is, routine bedsharing pairs slept in different rooms, routine solitary sleepers bedshared. All mothers and infants were healthy and nearly exclusively breastfeeding. The infants ranged in ages from 11 to 15 weeks (the peak age for SIDS).
We found that bedsharing doubled the number of nightly breastfeeds and tripled the total nightly duration of breastfeeding (see Figures 9 and 10). Bedsharing also correlated with shorter average intervals between breastfeeding sessions. Among our 70 nearly exclusively breastfeeding mothers, we found that the average interval between breastfeeds was approximately an hour and a half on the bedsharing night-the approximate length of the mothers' (adult) sleep cycle. That is, infant nighttime nutritional needs and feeding cycle while cosleeping correlated with the general length of the ultradian (subcycle of sleep) sleep cycle (90-120 minutes) of the human adult-a correlation never before observed or proposed. When sleeping in separate bedrooms (but still within earshot), the breastfeeding interval was at least twice as long.42
The supine position is the universal sleep position for infants, having evolved specifically to facilitate and make possible nighttime breastfeeding. Indeed, our studies reveal that without instruction, routinely bedsharing breastfeeding mothers practically always placed their infants in the safe supine position, probably because it is difficult, if not impossible, to breastfeed a prone sleeping infant. From our infrared video studies of bedsharing mothers and infants, it appears that supine infant sleep maximizes the infant's overall ability to control its microenvironment, and especially to elicit breastfeeds.43,44 In addition to permitting the infant to move toward and away from the breast, back sleeping permits the infant to remove blankets covering its face, turn to face toward or away from the mother, touch its face, wipe its nose, and, without a great deal of effort, suck on its fist or fingers, thus making loud sounds that will awaken its mother, who often then breastfeeds the infant (see Figure 10).
Our studies also suggest that supine infant sleep in the breastfeeding/bedsharing context maximizes the chances of the baby detecting and responding in synchrony with the mother's movements, sounds, and touches, and vice versa.45,46,47 The supine position of the infant promotes easy and constant communication between infant and mother, thus furthering mutual attachment and trust (a prerequisite for healthy infant development); in addition, it may stimulate the infant, through olfactory cues, to want to breastfeed more frequently, therein further suppressing the mother's ovulation. This model constitutes yet another reason to view the mother-infant relationship not simply in terms of how mothers regulate their infants, but rather how mothers and infants mutually regulate each other's physiology, including the mother's reproductive status.
The increased breastfeeding that accompanies bedsharing raises the possibility of enhanced immunological protection for the infant from potentially dangerous bacteria and viruses. Because bedsharing in the context of a breastfeeding mother leads to the use of the single most important defense against SIDS, the supine infant sleep position, we argued that the combination of breastfeeding and bedsharing may provide and enhance potentially significant health gains for the baby and nonsmoking mother alike, including reducing the infant's chances of dying from SIDS. Indeed, since the back-to-sleep campaign in 1992, which no doubt largely accounts for the significant reduction of SIDS to the present, breastfeeding rates have increased to historic highs (see Figure 11). If, as studies indicate, breastfeeding promotes the choice to bedshare, and more American parents are bedsharing than ever before, then perhaps these practices have also contributed to the reduction of SIDS since 1992. Most American breastfeeding mothers do not smoke and have access to safety information. Hence, the American situation of high breastfeeding, high supine infant sleep, reduced maternal smoking among this group, and safe bedsharing could well parallel the situation in Japan, discussed above, and as reported in Figures 2-5.
Infant-Parent Sleep Difficulties
Because infant sleep biology changes much more slowly than cultural values, sleep environments that are optimal for infants may not be the ones encouraged by the culture. Moreover, widely accepted infant sleep management strategies may be sufficient for some infants and children but unsuitable for others. Some families may apply norms established for bottle-fed, solitary-sleeping infants to their own children when it is inappropriate to do so, leading parents to conclude either that their parenting skills are deficient or that their child is uncooperative.
Ironically, this situation best describes what occurs in developed countries such as the US, Great Britain, and Australia, where as many as one out of every three otherwise healthy children may have problems falling or staying asleep, after having first been conditioned to sleep alone.48 Rather than infant or caregiver deficiencies, such high percentages probably reflect overconfidence in the validity of our definitions and expectations about how infants should sleep, and the rigidity with which parents interpret and apply messages offered by health professionals.
Indeed, parents' rigid expectations concerning how their infants should sleep can be used to predict the likelihood that infant/child sleep problems will manifest: The more rigid the expectations, the more likely it is that parents will report dissatisfaction with their child's sleep behavior.49 Night awakenings constitute a problem only for parents who expect their children to sleep through the night.
It is only in the last century or so, and in a relatively small number of cultures, that parents and health professionals have become concerned with how infants should be conditioned to sleep. And only in Western cultures are infants thought to need to "learn" to sleep, in this case alone and without parental contact. Most cultures simply take infant sleep for granted.
The Cultural/Scientific Bias against Cosleeping
It has been easy for public officials to conclude that the problems associated with cosleeping are not worth solving, in part because of our society's unique cultural history. In popular parenting books and childcare magazines, cosleeping may be (1) described as if it were a homogenous concept, (2) ignored completely, or (3) presented in terms of the likely or inevitable "problems" that could arise, especially the danger of suffocation. Sometimes cosleeping is explicitly discouraged; at other times the message is subtler. The most frequently cited reasons for recommending separate sleeping quarters for parents and children include preservation of the marriage; promotion of the child's individualism and autonomy; avoidance of incest and suffocation; promotion of the child's social competence; and strengthening of the child's gender and sexual identities.
Indeed, where a problem or potential problem with cosleeping can be identified, rather than being considered simply something to be solved, it becomes an argument against the practice, as if all families who cosleep will experience the same problem. Furthermore, problems associated with cosleeping are presented as if they cannot be solved in the same manner as, for example, problems associated with solitary sleep.
Throughout the literature, cosleeping is described as the cause of marital discord, although data from Sweden refute this notion.50 Cosleeping is also cited as the cause of sibling jealousy; while possibly true, it is probably only one of many causes. Parents are warned that cosleeping creates a "bad habit," one that is "difficult to break." Cosleeping is said to confuse the infant or child emotionally or sexually, or to induce overstimulation: "Sleeping in your bed can make your child feel confused and anxious rather than relaxed and reassured. Even a young toddler may find this repeated experience overly stimulating."51 But no evidence is offered to show how, when, and under what circumstances this happens; nor is there any acknowledgment that perhaps understimulation could be a more serious clinical and psychological problem.
A child needs to sleep alone, it is said, in order to establish a lifetime of good sleep hygiene, as well as to create a sense of self and comfort with aloneness, skills that presumably foster self-reliance and a strong sexual identity, all "moral goods." Again, not only is no evidence presented that supports these statements, but new evidence from a number of studies shows the opposite. In fact, when bedsharing occurs in the context of ongoing healthy social relationships, toddlers and children are more independent, not less, and when they're older, they have stronger sexual identities, not weaker ones, and are able to handle stress better (see Figure 7). Scientific paradigms do not change quickly or easily. The concept of infant-parent cosleeping is not readily assimilated by those who have spent their scientific lives documenting the normality of solitary infant sleep and accepting uncritically the alleged deleterious consequences of cosleeping. Probably few researchers, clinicians, and parents routinely coslept with their own parents, a factor that would strongly influence their comfort with the practice. Perhaps an appreciation of diverse childcare practices, including cosleeping, will come only with the growing populations of non-European immigrants in Western countries. As demographics on that score suggest, the question is not if the paradigm will change, but how soon.
Conclusions and Recommendations
The vast majority of scientific studies on infant behavior and development conducted in diverse fields during the last 100 years suggest that the question placed before us should not be "Is it safe to sleep with my baby?" but rather, "Is it safe not to do so?" An objective reading of the CPSC's own database leads to a very different conclusion than the one it reached-namely, that no infant should sleep outside of the supervision and company of a responsible adult caregiver. The issue is too complex to recommend in a sweeping way that all families should bedshare; still, any public safety campaign should recommend that at the very least every infant should be placed, preferably within arm's reach, sleeping on a different surface, alongside a responsible adult caregiver. Room- sharing alone reduces the infant's chances of dying from SIDS fourfold, according to the largest epidemiological study of SIDS yet undertaken.52
Recall that, until recent history, nighttime breastfeeding and infant and maternal cosleeping functioned in tandem in all societies, and that both patterns remain an inevitable and inseparable system for most people today, including a growing number of Western parents. When practiced safely, cosleeping with breastfeeding (whether bedsharing or not) represents a highly effective, adaptive, integrated childcare system that can enhance attachment, communication, nutrition, and infant immune efficiency thanks to the increased breastfeedings and the increased parental supervision and mutual affection that accompany this practice. Moreover, bedsharing and breastfeeding contribute indirectly to maternal and infant health by maximizing the intervals between succeeding births, therein lessening sibling competition for limited maternal resources. Cosleeping infants appear more content than those who sleep (or try to sleep) by themselves. With increased maternal contact and feeding, crying is significantly reduced, and, contrary to conventional thinking, maternal and infant sleep can be increased. Consequently, less energy is siphoned away from essential infant activities such as growth and defense against infectious disease.
As renowned child psychotherapist D. Winnicott said half a century ago, "There is no such thing as a baby; there is a baby and someone." Perhaps no childcare practice better reflects this truth than that of a human infant sleeping and breastfeeding next to its mother's body, enjoying her loving and protective responses. For these reasons, neither governmental regulatory agencies, associations of crib manufacturers, nor medical authorities, many of whom confuse their personal preferences and ideologies for science, will ever be able to deny parents and infants what they want to do naturally-and that is to sleep and feed side by side.
NOTES
1. For a review of scientific studies, see Touch in Early Development, T. Field, ed. (Mahway, New Jersey: Lawrence Earlbaum and Assoc., 1995).
2. J. J. McKenna, "An Anthropological Perspective on the Sudden Infant Death Syndrome (SIDS): The Role of Parental Breathing Cues and Speech Breathing Adaptations," Med. Anthrop. 10 (1986): 9-53.
3. J. J. McKenna and S. Mosko, "Mother Infant Cosleeping: Toward a New Beginning," in Sudden Infant Death Syndrome: Problems, Puzzles, Possibilities, R. Byard and H. Krous, eds. (New York: Arnold Publishing, 2001), 258-272.
4. J. Young and P. J. Fleming, "Reducing the Risks of SIDS: The Role of the Pediatrician." Paediatrics Today 6, no. 2 (1998): 41-48.
5. D. A. Drago and A. L. Dannenberg, "Infant Mechanical Suffocation Deaths in the United States, 1980-1997," Pediatrics 103, no. 5 (1999): e59.
6. S. Nakamura et al., "Review of Hazards Associated with Children Placed in Adult Beds," Arch. Pediat. Adolesc. Med. 153 (1999): 1018-1023.
7. N. J. Scheer, "Safe Sleeping Environments for Infants: A CPSC Perspective," Program and Abstracts, Sixth International SIDS Conference, Auckland, New Zealand, February 8-11, 2000.
8. See Note 1.
9. C. Richard et al., "Sleeping Position, Orientation, and Proximity in Bedsharing Infants and Mothers," Sleep 19 (1996): 667-684.
10. E. B. Thoman and S. E. Graham, "Self-Regulation of Stimulation by Premature Infants," Pediatrics 78 (1986): 855-860.
11. M. W. Stewart and L. A. Stewart, "Modification of Sleep Respiratory Patterns by Auditory Stimulation: Indications of Techniques for Preventing Sudden Infant Death Syndrome?" Sleep 14 (1991): 241-248.
12. A. F. Korner and E. B. Thoman, "The Relative Efficacy of Contact and Vestibular-Proprioceptive Stimulation on Soothing Neonates," Child Dev. 43 (1972): 443-453.
13. A. F. Korner et al., "Reduction of Sleep Apnea and Bradycardia in Pre-Term Infants on Oscillating Waterbeds: A Controlled Polygraphic Study," Pediatrics 61 (1978): 528-533.
14. A. H. Sankaran et al., "Sudden Infant Death Syndrome (SIDS) and Infant Care Practices in Saskatchewan, Canada," Program and Abstracts, Sixth SIDS International Conference, Auckland, New Zealand, February 8-11, 2000.
15. M. A. Kibel and M. F. Davies, "Should the Infant Sleep in Mother's Bed?" Program and Abstracts, Sixth SIDS International Conference, Auckland, New Zealand, February 8-11, 2000.
16. D. P. Davies, "Cot Death In Hong Kong: A Rare Problem?" The Lancet 2 (1985): 1346-1348.
17. N. P. Lee et al., "Sudden Infant Death Syndrome in Hong Kong: Confirmation of Low Incidence," British Medical Journal 298 (1999): 72.
18. S. Fukai and F. Hiroshi, "1999 Annual Report, Japan SIDS Family Association," Sixth SIDS International Conference, Auckland, New Zealand, 2000.
19. E. Wilson, "Sudden Infant Death Syndrome (SIDS) and Environmental Perturbations in Cross-Cultural Context," Master's thesis, University of Calgary (Alberta), 1990.
20. J. Yelland et al., "Explanatory Models about Maternal and Infant Health and Sudden Infant Death Syndrome among Asian-Born Mothers," in Asian Mothers, Australian Birth, Pregnancy, Childbirth, Child Rearing: The Asian Experience in an English-Speaking Country, P. L. Rice, ed. (Melbourne: Ausmeed Publications, 1996), 175- 189.
21. E. A. S. Nelson et al., "International Child Care Practice Study: Infant Sleeping Environment," Early Hum. Dev. 62 (2001): 43-55.
22. See Note 20.
23. See Note 19.
24. C. Carroll-Pankhurst and A. Mortimer, "Sudden Infant Death Syndrome, Bed-Sharing, Parental Weight, and Age at Death," Pediatrics 107, no. 3 (2001): 530-536.
25. Ibid.
Hi Dr. McKenna,
ReplyDeleteThanks for putting this informative article out there. I just wanted to further share with you that bedsharing is very common in certain parts of Spain, and there is some really useful data about in the social science literature if you are interested. I think that new mothers are lucky to have such an authoritative source of advice!
the best,
J.